n his book 10% Happier (2014), Dan Harris
observes that the little voice in our heads,
our internal narrator, can sometimes be
“creative, generous, or funny” and can at other
times be a “malicious puppeteer,” destroying our
self-confidence (p. viii). The key to happiness, he
argues, is redirecting this voice to become more
encouraging and focused.
The same might be said for students: Helping
them tame the voice in their heads while learning
can make all the difference.
Although teacher questioning techniques are
important to student success,
a large body of research suggests that good questions are
even more important within
the private world of students’
minds.
The concept of self-questioning while learning
has been around for quite
some time. Self-questioning
is considered a metacognitive
strategy. It’s the voice in our heads that assesses
where we are with our learning. It says things like,
Wait, I don’t get this.
Generating this voice is something effective
learners appear to do naturally. For example, if
you’re watching a science show on TV and hear
an astronomer explain that much of the starlight
in the night sky emanates from stars that may no
longer exist, a little voice in your head might say,
Wait, I don’t get that as you grab the remote and
back up the program.
Metacognitive self-questions help us connect
what we’re learning with our prior knowledge
(Oh, now I remember—starlight travels at the speed
of light), the process at the heart of all learning.
They can also help us look for big ideas that guide
our learning and connect it back to ourselves ( The
universe is unfathomably large and ever-expanding;
I feel really small now). And they can lead to other
questions that further guide learning (How exactly
do astronomers measure the distance of faraway
stars?).
A Strong Research Base
In 1991, Alison King set out to see whether self-
questioning, which had been demonstrated to
effectively support reading comprehension, could
have equally powerful effects on student compre-
hension of oral lectures. She wondered whether it
would be too distracting for
students to ask themselves
questions while listening to
a lecture. She also wondered
whether students could inter-
nalize the strategy.
King trained a class of 9th
graders to ask themselves
higher-order comparison-
contrast questions (like, How
are Shintoism and Buddhism
alike?); causal-relationship
questions (How did the rise
of the Shogun affect Japanese
development and culture?); and analysis questions
(Which king was best for England?) while listening
to history lectures. She also gave them “question
starters” to model the sort of questions they could
ask themselves during lectures and taught them
about metacognition, including how to ask them-
selves questions like, What do I still not understand
about this?
After these students listened to a series of lectures, one group of trained students used the questions they had generated in cooperative-learning
groups; another used the questions for individual
self-reflection. A third group, the control group,
had received no instruction on self-questioning
before the lectures. They simply reviewed the
lecture material independently or in unstructured
peer groups.
All students were tested on comprehension and
retention of the material immediately following
Generating self-
questions is
something effective
learners appear
to do naturally.
I
Bryan Goodwin with Heather Hein
RESEARCH SAYS
Help Students Train Their Inner Voice